The right parietal lobe is one of the four major lobes of the brain and is located towards the top and back of the brain. It plays a vital role in several functions of the body and is responsible for integrating information from various senses to give us a sense of spatial awareness and orientation.
One of the primary functions of the right parietal lobe is spatial awareness. It helps us to understand our position in space and our surroundings. The right parietal lobe works with other parts of the brain, such as the occipital lobe, to process visual information and create a spatial map of our environment. This map helps us navigate and move around in our environment.
In addition to spatial awareness, the right parietal lobe also plays a role in somatosensation. Somatosensation refers to our sense of touch, pressure, temperature, and pain. The right parietal lobe receives information from the somatosensory cortex, which is responsible for processing information related to touch, pressure, and temperature from different parts of the body. This information is then integrated into our perception of the world around us.
The right parietal lobe is also involved in the perception of time. Research has shown that patients with damage to the right parietal lobe have difficulty estimating the duration of time intervals. This suggests that the right parietal lobe may play a role in our ability to perceive time.
Another important function of the right parietal lobe is attention. The right parietal lobe is involved in both visual and auditory attention. It helps us to focus our attention on specific stimuli in our environment and filter out irrelevant information. This ability is essential for our daily functioning and helps us to stay focused and productive.
Studies have also suggested that the right parietal lobe plays a role in social cognition. Social cognition refers to our ability to understand the mental states of others and ourselves. The right parietal lobe has been shown to be involved in perspective-taking, which is the ability to understand the viewpoints of others. This suggests that the right parietal lobe may play a role in empathy and social interaction.
Damage to the right parietal lobe can lead to several deficits. Patients with damage to the right parietal lobe may experience difficulty with spatial awareness and may have trouble navigating their environment. They may also experience difficulty with somatosensation, such as difficulty feeling sensations in their limbs. Additionally, damage to the right parietal lobe can result in neglect syndrome, a condition where the patient ignores one side of their body or environment.
In conclusion, the right parietal lobe plays a vital role in several functions of the body, including spatial awareness, somatosensation, perception of time, attention, and social cognition. Damage to the right parietal lobe can lead to deficits in these functions, highlighting the importance of this brain region for our daily functioning.
Sources:
Cabeza, R. & Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(1), 1-47.
Karnath, H.O. (2001). New insights into the functions of the superior temporal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(8), 568-576.
Koch, G., Versace, V., Bonnì, S., Lupo, F., Lo Gerfo, E., Oliveri, M., . . . Caltagirone, C. (2010). Resonance magnetic stimulation of the precuneus enhances memory and neural activity in prodromal Alzheimer’s disease. NeuroImage, 51(2), 1079-1085.
Lee, M.S., Ernst, E. (2009). Acupuncture for pain: An overview of Cochrane reviews. Chinese Journal of Integrative Medicine, 15(2), 148-157.
Luauté, J., Halligan, P., Rode, G., Rossetti, Y., Boisson, D. (2006). Visuo-spatial neglect: A systematic review of current interventions and their effectiveness. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(4), 961-982.
Molenberghs, P., Sale, M.V., Mattingley, J.B. (2012). Is there a critical lesion site for unilateral spatial neglect? A meta-analysis using activation likelihood estimation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 78.
Mort, D.J., Malhotra, P., Mannan, S.K., Rorden, C., Pambakian, A., Kennard, C., . . . Husain, M. (2003). The anatomy of visual neglect. Brain, 126(Pt 9), 1986-1997.
Ohnishi, T., Matsuda, H., Tabira, T., Asada, T., Uno, M., & Katoh, M. (2001). Changes in brain morphology in Alzheimer disease and normal aging: Is Alzheimer disease an exaggerated aging process? American Journal of Neuroradiology, 22(9), 1680-1685.
Paulesu, E., Frith, C.D., Frackowiak, R.S., & The Neural Basis of Mirror Reading. Brain, 115(1), 267-271.
Reuter-Lorenz, P.A., Jonides, J., Smith, E.E., Hartley, A., Miller, A., Marshuetz, C., & Koeppe, R.A. (2000). Age differences in the frontal lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory revealed by PET. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(1), 174-187.